2% ± 1 2% 32 This study analyzed both kinetic and stride characte

2% ± 1.2%.32 This study analyzed both kinetic and stride characteristics of runners in minimalist, as well as traditional

shoes, both at the beginning and end of a 50-km run through the collection of pressure data, sEMG recordings, and limited 3D motion capture Selleck Anti-cancer Compound Library data. Of significance, the runners in this study who adopted a more posterior initial contact area after the 50-km run were those more closely associated with muscle fatigue of the gastrocnemius as defined by the theory of Wakeling et al.,30 which may accompany long-distance, sustained velocity running. In addition, peak pressures were significantly greater in the minimalist shoe type, specifically in the medial forefoot, which may predispose to an increased risk of metatarsal stress fractures in the

setting of improper training. Due to the limited study size of only FFS runners, the ability to generalize to all runners of varying foot-strike patterns must be cautioned. ALK inhibitor drugs Additional studies are necessary to (a) validate the observed findings of altered gait pattern, pressure data, and stride characteristics as a result of fatigue in both shoe type conditions; (b) further investigate the applicability of the isometric, constant force contraction theory in a dynamic, endurance exercise, such as running; and (c) further investigate the proposed theory of change in motor unit recruitment etiology observed during sustained, submaximal activity, such as endurance running. This study was supported, in part, by the Medical College of Wisconsin’s Department of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, as well as by grant 1UL1RR031973

from the Clinical and Translational Science Award (CTSA) program of the National Center for Research Resources, National Institutes of Health. “
“Running is becoming an increasingly popular activity among Americans with over 50 million participants. This represents a growth of almost 8% in 1 year and a 57% increase in the last 10 years.1 More people are running either for fitness or performance with almost 14 million US road race participants in 2011, a 7% increase from Iodothyronine deiodinase the year prior. All these runners are creating a huge market for running gear as running shoe sales topped 2.46 billion dollars in 2011 with over 65% of runners spending more than 90 dollars on their running shoes.1 Running shoes have become increasingly more expensive with more technology and research behind the design of modern running shoes. However, running injuries appear to be just as prevalent as they always have been with an estimated 30%–75% of average recreational runners becoming injured at least once each year.2 and 3 Despite increasing money and technology invested into shoe design, there has yet to be a decrease in running injury rates per capita.2 Humans have run minimally shod or barefoot for millions of years, but only recently has the running shoe become an essential part of a runner’s gear.

1_C169TAG (Figure 3A), which reduced the number of plasmids neede

1_C169TAG (Figure 3A), which reduced the number of plasmids needed for transfection and allowed tracking the location of PIRK channels. Fusion of GFP to the C terminus of Kir2.1 was shown previously to not affect Kir2.1 channel physiology (Sekar et al., 2007). Addition of Cmn to the bath resulted

in fluorescently labeled HEK293T cells (Figure 3B; Figure S2A) and the expression of full-length Kir2.1-GFP fusion protein (Figure S2B). A brief (1 s) pulse of UV light (385 nm LED, 40 mW/cm2) led to activation of an inwardly rectifying current that was blocked by Ba2+ (Figures 3C and 3D). The activation kinetics had fast and slow components with time constants (τ) of 298 ± 134 ms and 15.0 ± 4.3 s, respectively (n = 7). Note that the amplitude of light-activated current is larger than that in Figure 2H, indicating that selleckchem PIRK expression level increased with the two plasmid system. When incorporated with Leu, Kir2.1_C169TAGLeu channels showed large IKir (8.30 ± 1.48 nA, n = 7), which was not affected by light illumination (data not shown). On the other hand, HEK293T cells expressing PIRK (Kir2.1_C169TAGCmn) channels produced no or negligible IKir before UV light (0.14 ± 0.07 nA, n = 10 versus 0.05 ± 0.02 nA, n = 9 for untransfected; p > 0.05, unpaired t test) and a marked increase in IKir after UV light (1.65 ± 0.41 nA, n = 10) (Figure 3E). The smaller Topoisomerase inhibitor IKir for

PIRK compared to Kir2.1_C169TAGLeu was likely due to the less efficient aminoacylation with CmnRS and, therefore, less Cmn incorporation. To investigate the relationship between the light dosage and current activation, we varied the duration and frequency of UV light pulses. Single

light pulses with different lengths were applied to cells expressing PIRK channels. Using a 40 mW/cm2 LED light Linifanib (ABT-869) source, 1 s and 500 ms light pulses evoked similar amounts of current at −100 mV (2.27 ± 0.51 nA, n = 5 for 1 s; 2.04 ± 0.39 nA, n = 5 for 500 ms). Shorter UV pulses (200 ms, 100 ms, and 50 ms) led to progressively smaller currents (Figure 3F). No significant change in current amplitude was measured with a single 20 ms light pulse (n = 6; data not shown). We next investigated the effect of sequential UV light pulses. Sequentially delivered light pulses of 200 ms duration each led to stepwise activation of PIRK channels (Figure 3G). Fewer UV pulses were required to maximally activate PIRK channels with UV light pulses of longer duration (Figure 3H). Together, these results illustrate that modulating the duration and number of light pulses can be used to fine-tune the extent of PIRK current activation. A significant obstacle in using Uaa technology has been the implementation of Uaa in vertebrate neurons. We therefore investigated the expression of PIRK channels in primary cultures of hippocampal neurons.

Regardless, correlations between transglutaminase activity and MT

Regardless, correlations between transglutaminase activity and MT stability in WT and TG2 KO mouse models are consistent with our hypothesis that transglutaminase activity is a major contributor to stable MT formation in vivo. Eighth, transglutaminase activity and TG2 protein levels correlate with MT stability during development and maturation in vivo. As axons mature and neuronal connections stabilize in response to selleck various postnatal modifiers, such as myelination, cold/Ca2+-insoluble tubulin levels increase (Kirkpatrick and Brady, 1994; Kirkpatrick

et al., 2001). Correspondingly, transglutaminase protein levels and enzymatic activity are elevated (Figure 9). This suggests that developmental regulation of transglutaminase contributes to MT stabilization as the brain matures. The role for transglutaminase activity and polyamines in stabilization of axonal MTs does not preclude their playing other roles in the nervous system. Transglutaminases are proposed to be involved in neuronal development (Bailey and Johnson, 2004; Maccioni and Seeds, 1986; Mahoney et al., 2000; Tucholski et al., 2001) and signaling (Basso et al., 2012; Dai et al., 2008; Facchiano et al., 2010), as well as in neurodegenerative diseases (Bailey et al., 2005; Basso et al., 2012; De Vivo et al., 2009; Ruan and Johnson, 2007). Transglutaminase activity

and polyamine levels correlate with brain maturation, neuronal differentiation, and learn more formation of neurites (Bailey and Johnson, 2004), but the underlying mechanisms are unclear. Here we propose that a key pathway for regulating neuronal development is modulation of MT stability by TG2-catalyzed polyamination of tubulins. Increases in both TG2 protein and Bay 11-7085 transglutaminase activity increased stable MTs in postnatal brains (Figure 9), concurrent with myelination and stabilization of neuronal circuitry. Molecular pathways responsible for effects

of myelination on TG2 protein level and transglutaminase activity are under investigation. Transglutaminase activity may also contribute to changes in cellular morphology (Gentile et al., 1992). Depletion of polyamines results in the disappearance of actin and MT bundles (Pohjanpelto et al., 1981). Inhibition of polyamine biosynthesis results in defects of neuronal morphogenesis, whereas exogenous polyamines stimulate adult neurogenesis (Malaterre et al., 2004). All these are consistent with our findings and suggest a common pathway based in part on alteration of MT dynamics and stability through polyamination of tubulin by TG2. Stabilizing MTs by transglutaminase and polyamines has many positive aspects for normal cytoskeletal structure and function in developing brain, but may be a double-edged sword in the aging nervous system. Transglutaminase activity and polyamine levels increase in the aging brain (Lesort et al.

The logic of the task was that a dependence on model-based or mod

The logic of the task was that a dependence on model-based or model-free strategies predicts different patterns by which feedback obtained after the second stage should impact future first-stage choices. We first considered stay-switch behavior as a minimally constrained approach to dissociate model-based and model-free control. A model-free reinforcement learning strategy predicts a main effect of reward on stay probability. This is because

model-free choice works without considering structure in the environment; hence, rewarded choices are more likely to be repeated, regardless of whether that reward followed a common or rare transition. A reward after an uncommon transition would therefore adversely increase the value of the chosen first-stage cue without updating the value of the unchosen cue. In contrast, under a model-based strategy, we expect a crossover interaction between the two factors, because a GABA cancer rare transition inverts the effect of a subsequent reward (Figure 1C). Under model-based control, receiving a reward after an uncommon transition increases the propensity to switch. This is because the rewarded second-stage stimulus can be more reliably accessed by choosing the rejected first-stage cue than by choosing the same cue again.

Using repeated-measures ANOVA, we examined the probability of staying or switching at the first stage dependent on drug state (L-DOPA or placebo), reward on previous trial (reward B-Raf inhibition or no reward), and transition type on previous trial (common or uncommon) (see Figure 2A). A significant main effect of reward, F(1,17) = 23.3, p < 0.001, demonstrates a model-free component in behavior (i.e., reward increases stay probability regardless of the transition type). A significant interaction between reward and transition, Etomidate F(1,17) = 9.75, p =

0.006, reveals a model-based component (i.e., subjects also take the task structure into account). These results show both a direct reinforcement effect (model-free) and an effect of task structure (model-based) and replicate previous findings ( Daw et al., 2011). The key analyses here concerned whether L-DOPA modulated choice propensities. Critically, we observed a significant drug × reward × transition interaction, F(1,17) = 9.86, p = 0.006, reflecting increased model-based behavior under L-DOPA treatment. We also observed a main effect of the drug, F(1,17) = 7.04, p = 0.017, showing that subjects are less perseverative under L-DOPA treatment. Interactions between drug and transition, F(1,17) = 4.09, p = 0.06, or drug and reward (which would indicate a drug-induced change in model-free control), F(1,17) = 1.10, p = 0.31, were not significant. Figure 2B shows the difference in stay probability between drug states corrected for a main effect of drug.

Neurons were recorded in area V4 in two rhesus macaques Experime

Neurons were recorded in area V4 in two rhesus macaques. Experimental and surgical procedures have been described previously (Reynolds et al., 1999). All procedures were approved by the Salk Institute Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and conformed to NIH guidelines. See Supplemental Experimental Procedures for further details. Stimuli were presented on a computer monitor (Sony Trinitron Multiscan, TC, 640 × 480 pixel resolution, 120 Hz) placed 57 cm from the eye. Eye position was continuously monitored with

an infrared eye tracking system (240 Hz, ETL-400; ISCAN). Experimental control was handled by NIMH Cortex software (http://www.cortex.salk.edu/). Trials were aborted if eye position deviated more that 1° from fixation. At the beginning of each recording session, neuronal RFs were mapped to determine the approximate spatial extent over which stimuli elicited BAY 73-4506 solubility dmso a visual response. Monkeys fixated a central point during which each neuron’s RF was mapped using subspace reverse correlation in which Gabor (eight orientations,

80% luminance contrast, spatial frequency 1.2 cpd, Gaussian half-width 2°) or ring stimuli (80% luminance contrast) appeared at 60 Hz. Each stimulus appeared at a random location selected from a 19 × 15 grid with 1° spacing in the inferior right visual field. The orientation see more of the Gabor stimuli and the color of all stimuli (one of six colors or achromatic) were randomly selected. This resulted in an estimate of the spatial RF, orientation, and color preference RNASEH2A of each neuron. Recordings were often made from multiple electrodes, and the preferences of units on separate channels did not always match. The stimuli for the main experiment were centered on the estimated

spatial RF of the best-isolated units. The monkey began each trial by fixating a central point for 200 ms and then maintained fixation through the trial. Each trial lasted 3 s, during which neuronal responses to a fast-reverse correlation sequence (16 ms stimulus duration, exponential distributed delay between stimuli with mean delay of 16 ms, i.e., 0 ms delay p = 1/2, 16 ms delay p = 1/4, 32 ms delay p = 1/8, and so on) were recorded. The stimuli were composed of oriented bars (eight orientations) or bar composites (16 orientations × 5 conjunction angles, total of 72 unique stimuli, Figure 1A). These latter stimuli were constructed from the conjunction of three bars at conjunction angles of 0°, 22.5°, 45°, 67.5°, and 90° between the end elements and the center. The five conjunction levels created five categories of shapes. These were enumerated as 0 (zero curvature/straight), 1 (low curvature), 2 (medium curvature), 3 (high curvature), and 4 (C).

Objects were scaled to be as large as possible while maintaining

Objects were scaled to be as large as possible while maintaining their aspect ratio and superimposed on a background consisting of noise of uniformly distributed intensity. Three sets of stimuli were generated by superimposing

several familiar and unfamiliar objects over an intact scene, a scrambled scene, or a scene that had been filtered to preserve general intensity patterns while removing spatial boundary information. All blocks consisted of 16 images, except for the latter three sets, which consisted of eight. All images subtended approximately 23° × 15°. During Entinostat recording, stimuli were presented for 100 ms, followed by a blank screen for 100 ms. Order was randomized. The

stimulus set consisted of 16 images each of familiar scenes, scrambled scenes, and textures, 15 images of familiar objects, 18 images of unfamiliar scenes, and a single image of uniform noise. Stimuli subtended approximately 55° Veliparib order × 39° in order to provide an immersive visual display. However, a control experiment showed no significant difference in scene selectivity when the same stimuli were shown at 46° × 32° or 35° × 24° (p = 0.70, Friedman’s test). Surface reconstruction based on anatomical volumes was performed using FreeSurfer (Massachusetts General Hospital) after skull stripping using FSL’s Brain Extraction Tool (University of Oxford). After applying these tools, segmentation was further refined manually. Analysis of functional click here volumes was performed using the FreeSurfer Functional Analysis Stream (Massachusetts General Hospital). Volumes were corrected for motion

and undistorted based on acquired field map. Runs in which the norm of the residuals of a quadratic fit of displacement during the run exceeded 5 mm and the maximum displacement exceeded 0.55 mm were discarded. Our monkeys worked continuously throughout each scanning session before ceasing to fixate entirely, at which point we discarded the final run. The resulting data were analyzed using a standard general linear model. For the scene contrast, the average of all scene blocks was compared to the average of all nonscene blocks, ignoring the fractured scenes and outlined rooms. For the microstimulation contrast, the average of the blocks with concomitant stimulation was compared to the average of the blocks without stimulation. Regions of interest were defined based on activations that were consistently observed in the same anatomical regions across subjects in one-third of the runs. All time courses and bar graphs displayed were generated from the remaining two-thirds. To compute the response to each image in the stimulus set, we averaged the number of spikes over the time window from 100 ms to 250 ms after stimulus onset (LPP) or from 75 ms to 150 ms after stimulus onset (MPP).

, 2012; Hoeffer et al , 2011), was increased after TBS in stratum

, 2012; Hoeffer et al., 2011), was increased after TBS in stratum radiatum at CA1 area in WT and Paip2a−/− slices. However, the increase was bigger in Paip2a−/− slices ( Figure S2E), indicating an association of excessive stimulation of mRNA translation and impaired L-LTP after TBS in Paip2a−/− slices

(see Discussion). Long-term depression (LTD) elicited by application of DHPG (3,5-dihydroxyphenylglycine, an mGluR1/5 agonist) or by low-frequency stimulation (LFS) was not altered in Paip2a−/− slices ( Figures 1G and S2F, respectively). Taken together, these results show that the threshold for the induction of protein synthesis-dependent L-LTP is lowered in Paip2a−/− slices. In contrast, stronger stimulation (TBS) leads to L-LTP impairment, while LTD is not affected. Based on the electrophysiological results, we predicted that Paip2a−/− mice would exhibit Galunisertib enhanced learning and memory after weak training. We investigated this using the hidden version of the Morris

water maze task (MWM), a hippocampal-dependent task for spatial learning and memory ( Morris et al., 1982). Paip2a−/− and WT littermates were trained with a weak training protocol that consisted of only a single training trial per day to find a submerged platform, in contrast to the standard protocol of three trials per day ( Costa-Mattioli et al., 2005, 2007). Overall, the swim latencies were not different between WT and Paip2a−/− mice over 6 days of training; F(1, 14) = 2.5, p = 0.136, repeated-measures ANOVA. However, on the third training day, Paip2a−/− mice reached the hidden platform significantly

faster (WT: 57.13 ± VX809 7.31 s; Paip2a−/−: 37.9 ± 4.90 s, p < 0.05, Resminostat Student’s t test) than WT mice ( Figure 2A), indicating faster learning since there were no differences in swimming speed (WT: 15.93 ± 1.52 cm/s; Paip2a−/−: 17.13 ± 0.73 cm/s, p > 0.05), thigmotaxis (swimming near the pool wall; WT: 44.88% ± 4.42%; Paip2a−/−: 41.88% ± 5.38%, p > 0.05), or escape latency in the visible version of MWM (WT: 11.38 ± 2.12 s; Paip2a−/−: 12.25 ± 3.05 s, p > 0.05). A probe test performed 24 hr after 3 days of training confirmed superior spatial memory in Paip2a−/− mice. WT mice demonstrated no preference for the quadrant where the platform was located during the training sessions (target quadrant), whereas Paip2a−/− mice displayed a clear preference for the target quadrant and platform location ( Figures 2B and 2C, respectively), spent significantly more time in the target quadrant ( Figure 2B), and crossed the platform location significantly more than WT mice ( Figure 2C). In the probe test, after 6 days of training, both groups demonstrated similar quadrant occupancy and platform crossing ( Figures 2D and 2E, respectively). Next, we used an object-location memory task to assess spatial long-term memory (LTM) of Paip2a−/− mice.

, 2003; Shaw et al , 2004), CaMKK (Anderson et al , 2008; Hawley

, 2003; Shaw et al., 2004), CaMKK (Anderson et al., 2008; Hawley et al., 2005; Hurley et al., 2005; Woods et al., 2005), and TAK1 (Momcilovic et al., 2006). In the nervous system, however, LKB1 does not seem to serve as a kinase for AMPK, since AMPKα phosphorylation at T172 was not changed in LKB1 null mice (Barnes et al., 2007). A likely candidate is CaMKK, since it is highly expressed in the brain and it associates with AMPK α and β subunits (Anderson et al., 2008). If that is the case, it is possible that oligomeric Aβ42 itself modulates intracellular calcium levels, thereby activating CaMKK. It will be of interest to test whether intracellular calcium levels change with

oligomeric Aβ42 addition in neurons. Can mTOR Neratinib activity modulation be developed into a therapy for AD? This is an attractive idea, since there are already many FDA-approved drugs that were designed to target

the mTOR pathways for treating other progressive metabolic diseases. Although attractive, the idea appears too premature at the present time mainly because the role of the mTOR pathway in AD is not fully understood. For instance, some reported improvement in cognitive function and neuronal toxicity with Rapamycin administration (Berger et al., 2006; Bové et al., 2011; Caccamo et al., 2010; Khurana et al., 2006; Spilman et al., 2010), while others reported the opposite (Lafay-Chebassier et al., 2005). Similarly, the reports vary as to whether there is an all inhibition Decitabine chemical structure or activation of the mTOR pathway in AD mouse models and/or human cases (Caccamo et al., 2011; Ma et al., 2010). Our data indicate that there is a significant translational block early in FAD mice. This notion was also supported by a global transcriptome analysis via RNaseq, which demonstrated a dramatic reduction in transcripts for ribosomes and elongation factors in FAD compared to the wild-type mice (data not shown). It is possible that the use of different animal models at different ages in each study contributed to the opposite outcomes. It seems safe to surmise that before one

takes further steps to alter the mTOR pathway or AMPK activity in pursuit of a treatment for AD, more systematic and consistent analyses are necessary. In conclusion, our findings suggest that JNK3 activation is central to the development of AD pathology by exacerbating metabolic stress that is induced by Aβ42 accumulation. This study thus identifies JNK3 as a promising new target of therapeutic intervention for Alzheimer’s disease. Tissues from the frontal cortex were obtained through UCSD Experimental Neuropath Laboratory. FAD mice in B6/SJL F1 hybrid background were initially crossed with JNK3 in knockout mice in B6 background to obtain FAD:JNK3+/− and control nontransgenic:JNK3+/−. This study was approved by the IACUC of the Ohio State University.

Indeed, Neuron showcases just this type of interdisciplinary appr

Indeed, Neuron showcases just this type of interdisciplinary approach. We are tremendously

grateful to all the authors who brought their ideas and vision to these Perspectives. These pieces were proposed to the authors as “reviews with a point of view” and a chance for the authors to bring their voice and perspective to these topics. Our intention was to spark discussion and debate, and we hope that you find these essays interesting, thought provoking, and perhaps even inspiring. A capstone for this issue is the “Behind the Covers” feature. No issue of Neuron would be complete without its iconic cover. This, too, has been true back to Issue 1. In “Behind the Covers” we brought back from the archives a selection of covers that we, as editors selleck chemicals llc of the journal, have enjoyed as much for the creative efforts and personal stories behind them as for their beauty. In today’s age where the cover is usually a tiny thumbprint

of an image on a website (or a smartphone) and readers of the print issues are fewer and fewer, we are Luminespib sometimes asked, “Why bother with a cover at all?” The answer: the cover is an act of celebration! For the authors featured, it’s the crowning achievement and the cherry on the cake. It’s also exciting for all of us here at Cell Press—the scientific editors, our production and support staff, and everyone involved in bringing you a new issue—to close an issue of the journal, and we look forward to releasing its content to the world. There is always that moment of anticipation—“What will

they think?” Keep those cover submissions coming! And because we couldn’t pack all the content we wanted to share with you into an issue, on our website you may have noticed that in the roll-up to the Society for Neuroscience meeting these past six months, we have been featuring Adenosine triphosphate a paper from each year of the journal, spotlighting the author and original paper, and reflecting on how the field has evolved since (http://www.cell.com/neuron/25). It’s remarkable that the legacy and impact of so many Neuron papers can still be felt years on from their publication date, and choosing just one paper for each year was a daunting task. We would like to celebrate with all of you at SFN and have a number of events planned. Neuron will be represented in the Cell Press/Elsevier booth (#213). We’ll be celebrating by showcasing 25 years of the most exciting research in neuroscience. Please stop by to pick up your free copy of our two special issues—the anniversary review issue and the featured research issue, as well as our annual special collection “Best of Neuron.” You can also find copies of other Cell Press journals, including Cell, Cell Reports, Trends in Neurosciences, and Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

Surprisingly, many synapses in the central nervous system can sus

Surprisingly, many synapses in the central nervous system can sustain synaptic activity upon high-frequency stimulation (Kopp-Scheinpflug et al., 2008, Kraushaar and Jonas, 2000, Lorteije et al., 2009 and Rancz et al., 2007). In order to maintain the fidelity

of synaptic transmission, synaptic vesicles (SVs) are required to undergo fast recycling to prevent depletion of the SV pool (Fernández-Alfonso PCI-32765 manufacturer and Ryan, 2004 and Sudhof, 2004). Recently it has been reported that interfering with the function of endocytic proteins causes a fast, stimulation-frequency-dependent depression of SV exocytosis (Hosoi et al., 2009 and Kawasaki et al., 2000). As obvious explanation for such findings, the lack of release-ready SVs may be invoked due to absence of recycled SVs. However, some of these inhibitory effects developed so rapidly that they cannot be explained by the lack of release-ready SVs, since the reservoir of SVs should well be able to maintain release for longer periods. In this study we investigated vesicle exocytosis in cultured rat hippocampal neurons using synaptopHluorin (Miesenböck et al., 1998 and Sankaranarayanan

et al., 2000) in the presence of Folimycin, a potent and specific inhibitor of vesicular reacidification, that does not affect exo-endocytosis screening assay (Zhou et al., 2000). We demonstrate that upon mild stimulation no reuse of SVs occurs within 40 s and that recruitment of pre-existing Asenapine SVs is fast enough to meet the needs of a high release rate. However, under the influence of the specific endocytosis inhibitor Dynasore (Macia et al., 2006) or the inhibitor of clathrin-mediated endocytosis Pitstop 2 (von Kleist et al., 2011) we observed a clear stimulation-frequency-dependent release depression. This probably reflects interference of these inhibitors with the process of rapid clearance of exocytosed SV components from the synaptic release sites. This notion was corroborated by the observation of acute vesicular protein accumulation around the release site using

dual-color STED nanoscopy. To reliably measure the level of synaptic release depression, we quantified the amount of exocytosis upon different stimulation strengths using synaptopHluorin (spH) in cultured hippocampal neurons (Miesenböck et al., 1998 and Sankaranarayanan et al., 2000). At presynaptic terminals expressing spH, exocytosis of SVs evoked by electric field stimulation (via action potentials, APs) led to dequenching of spH molecules in neutral extracellular buffer, resulting in an instantaneous fluorescence increase (Figure 1A). Under such experimental conditions, the fluorescence change is proportional to the amount of spH exocytosed. The absolute amplitude of the signal can differ, however, from cell to cell due to inhomogeneous expression of the probe and variation in release probability.